Kirish Roʻyxatdan oʻtish

Docx

  • Referatlar
  • Diplom ishlar
  • Boshqa
    • Slaydlar
    • Referatlar
    • Kurs ishlari
    • Diplom ishlar
    • Dissertatsiyalar
    • Dars ishlanmalar
    • Infografika
    • Kitoblar
    • Testlar

Dokument ma'lumotlari

Narxi 12000UZS
Hajmi 138.3KB
Xaridlar 0
Yuklab olingan sana 16 Aprel 2024
Kengaytma docx
Bo'lim Kurs ishlari
Fan Ingliz tili

Sotuvchi

Bohodir Jalolov

Grammatical categories of the verb

Sotib olish
1THE   MINISTRY   OF   HIGHER   EDUCATION, SCIENCE   AND
INNOVATIONS OF THE REPUBLIC OF UZBEKISTAN
SAMARKAND   STATE   INSTITUTE   OF   FOREIGN   LANGUAGES   FACULTY
OF ENGLISH PHILOLOGY AND TRANSLATION
SUBJECT :
COURSE   WORK
THEME:  Grammatical   categories   of   the   verb
SAMARKAND   2024 2CONTENT
INTRODUCTION ................................................................................................... 3
CHAPTER   I.   INTRODUCTION   TO   VERBS ...................................................... 6
1.1. Definition   of  verbs ............................................................................................. 6
1.2. Importance   of   verbs   in   sentence   construction .................................................. 19
CONCLUSION   ON   CHAPTER   I ........................................................................ 25
CHAPTER   II.   THE   SYSTEM   OF   YHE   VERB   IS   REPRESENTED   BY   TWO   SETS
OF   FORMS   FINITE   AND   NON- FINITE ....................................................................... 26
2.1. The   non-finite   forms:   infinitive,   gerund,   present   participle ............................. 26
2.2. The verb in its finite form possesses the number, tense, aspect, voice and  
mood ........................................................................................................................ 29
CONCLUSION   ON   CHAPTER  II ...................................................................... 37
CONCLUSION ...................................................................................................... 38
THE   LIST   OF   USED  LITERATURE ................................................................. 39 3INTRODUCTION
Grammatically  the  verb  is  the most  complex  part  of  speech.   This  is  due  to
the   central   role   it   performs   in   the   expression   of   the   predicative   functions   of   the
sentence,   i.e.   the   functions   establishing   the   connection   between   the   situation
(situational event) named in the utterance and reality. A verb is often defined as a
word which shows action or state of being.   The verb is one of the principle parts of
the   sentence.   Recognizing   the   verb   is   often   the   most   important   step   in
understanding the meaning of the sentence. From the morphological point of view
the verb is characterised by a complex system  of categories the main of which is
the   category   of   tense.   It   serves   for   expression   of   time-distinctions.   With   this
category presents  the most  troublesome  problems for  a foreign student  to master.
So this category should be   thoroughly investigated to escape   problems   with the   use
the right verb form in   one or another   situation. In the intricate tapestry of   language,
verbs   stand   as   dynamic   components,   bearing   the   responsibility   of   conveying
action,   states,   and   relationships   within   sentences.   Central   to   linguistic   analysis,
verbs   possess   a   myriad   of   grammatical   categories   that   intricately   shape   the
structure and meaning of utterances. From   the expression of tense and aspect to the
portrayal   of   mood   and   voice,   the   grammatical   categories   of   the   verb   offer   a
window into the intricate workings of language systems across the globe. Also this
research   endeavors   to   delve   deep   into   the   multifaceted   nature   of   verb   categories,
probing   the   underlying   principles   that   govern   their   manifestation   and   usage   in
diverse   linguistic   contexts.   By   embarking   on   this   journey,   we   aim   to   unravel   the
complexities   inherent   in   verb   categorization,   shedding   light   on   both   universal
patterns   and   language-specific   nuances.   The   President   of   our   country   Shavkat
Miromonovich   Mirziyoyev   is   also   creating   excellent   conditions   for   our   youth   to
get   a   good   education.   Under   the   leadership   of   our   President,   great   changes   are
taking   place   in   education,   art,   culture   and   social   life   in   our   country.   In   an   era
dominated   by   digital   communication,   the   ability   to   convey   thoughts,   ideas,   and
emotions effectively has become more crucial than ever [1,91]. 4Through a comprehensive examination of theoretical frameworks, empirical
data,   and   practical   applications,   this   study   seeks   to   contribute   to   the   broader
understanding   of   language   structure   and   communication.   Furthermore,   it   endeavors
to   elucidate   the   practical   implications   of   verb   categorization   in   fields   such   as
theoretical   linguistics,   computational   linguistics,   and   language   teaching.   As   we
embark   on   this   exploration   of   the   grammatical   categories   of   the   verb,   we   are
poised to uncover   insights that not only deepen our   comprehension of   language   but
also pave the way for advancements in various domains where linguistic analysis
plays a pivotal role.
Actuality of research   on the grammatical categories of the verb lies in its pivotal
role   in   understanding   language   structure   and   communication   across   diverse
linguistic   contexts.   With   ongoing   developments   in   theoretical   linguistics,
computational   linguistics,   and   language   teaching   methodologies,   there   is   a   growing
need to delve deeper into the   intricacies of   verb categories.   This research addresses
fundamental   questions   about   how   verbs   encode   temporal,   aspectual,   modal,   and
voice-related   information,   contributing   to   advancements   in   language   analysis,
machine   translation,   natural   language   processing,   and   language   pedagogy.
Moreover,   it   facilitates   cross-linguistic   comparisons   and   sheds   light   on   the
universal principles underlying verb categorization, making it an area of enduring
scholarly interest and practical significance.
The   purpose   of   this   course   work  on   the   grammatical categories   of the   verb could
be to deepen understanding and analysis of the various features that verbs possess
within   different   languages.   This   may   involve   exploring   how   verbs   express   tense,
aspect,   mood,   voice,   and   other   grammatical   categories,   and   how   these   categories
function   within   sentence   structure   and   communication.   Additionally,   the
coursework may delve into theoretical frameworks for categorizing and describing
verb   forms,   as   well   as   practical   applications   such   as   language   teaching   or
computational linguistics. 5The subject  of  research   on the grammatical  categories of  the verb encompasses
an   exploration   of   the   various   features   and   functions   of   verbs   within   different
linguistic frameworks. This involves investigating how verbs express tense,   aspect,
mood,   voice,   and   other   grammatical   categories,   and   analyzing   their   role   in
sentence   structure   and   communication.   By   examining   theoretical   models,   empirical
data, and practical applications, researchers seek to deepen understanding of verb
categorization   across   languages,   uncover   universal   patterns,   and   advance
knowledge   in  fields   such   as   theoretical   linguistics,   computational   linguistics,   and
language teaching.
The   object   of   this   course   work   on   the   grammatical   categories   of   the   verb   is   to
investigate and analyze the diverse features and functions of verbs within various
linguistic   contexts.   This   includes   examining   how   verbs   express   tense,   aspect,
mood,   voice,   and   other   grammatical   categories,   and   how   these   categories
contribute   to  the   overall   structure   and  meaning   of   sentences.   Through   theoretical
exploration   and   practical   application,   the   coursework   aims   to   deepen
comprehension   and   proficiency   in   understanding   and   utilizing   verb   forms   within
language.
Structure of the course work  the content of the work is reflected in the structure
of   the   work,   which   consists   of   an   introduction,   chapters,   conclusion,   the   list   of used
literature.   The   introduction   substantiates   the   relevance   of   the   chosen   research   topic;
formulates   the   goal   and   objectives; defines   the   object   and   subject   of   study;   presents
the methodology and basic research methods; reveals scientific   novelty, theoretical
significance   and   practical   value   of   the   work.   The   first   chapter   analyses   the   the
Introduction   to   Verbs.   The   second   chapter   analyses   the   system   of   the   verb   is
represented by two sets of forms finite and non-finite forms. 6CHAPTER   I.   INTRODUCTION   TO   VERBS .
1.1. Definition   of   verbs
Grammatically   the   verb   is   the   most   complex   part   of   speech.   First   of   all   it
performs the central role in realizing predication - connection between situation in
the   utterance   and   reality.   That   is   why   the   verb   is   of   primary   informative
significance in an utterance. Besides, the verb possesses quite a lot of grammatical
categories. Furthermore, within the class of verb various subclass  divisions based
on different principles of classification can befound. Semantic features of the verb.
The verb possesses the grammatical meaning of verbiality - the ability to denote a
process   developing   in   time.   This   meaning   is   inherent   not   only   in   the   verbs   denoting
processes,   but   also   in   those   denoting   states,   forms   of   existence,   evaluations,   etc.
Morphological features of the verb. The verb possesses the following grammatical
categories:   tense,   aspect,   voice,   mood,   person,   number,   finitude   and   phase.   The
common   categories   for   finite   and   non-finite   forms   are   voice,   aspect,   phase   and
finitude.   The   grammatical   categories   of   the   English   verb   find   their   expression   in
synthetical   and   analytical   forms.   The   formative   elements   expressing   these
categories   are   grammatical   affixes,   inner   inflexion   and   function   words.   Some
categories   have   only   synthetical   forms   (person,   number),   others   -   only   analytical
(voice).   There   are   also   categories   expressed   by   both   synthetical   and   analytical
forms   (mood,   tense,   aspect).   Syntactic   features.   The   most   universal   syntactic
feature   of   verbs   is   their   ability  to  be   modified   by   adverbs.   The   second   important
syntactic criterion is the ability of the verb to perform the syntactic function of the
predicate.   However,   this   criterion   is   not   absolute   because   only   finite   forms   can
perform   this   function   while   non-finite   forms   can   be   used   in   any   function   but
predicate. And finally, any verb in the form of the infinitive can be combined with
a modal verb [1,17].
Classifications   of   English   verbs
According   to   different   principles   of   classification,   classifications   can   be
morphological, lexical-morphological, syntactical and functional. 7A. Morphological   classifications
I. According to their stem-types all verbs fall into: simple ( to go), sound-replacive
(food-to   feed,   blood-to   bleed),   stress-replacive   (import   -   to   import,   transport-to
transport,   expanded   (with   the   help   of   suffixes   and   prefixes):   cultivate,   justify,
overcome,  composite  (correspond   to  composite  nouns):  to  blackmail),  phrasal:  to
have   a   smoke,   to   give   a   smile   (they   always   have   an   ordinary   verb   as   an   equivalent).
2.According   to   the   way   of   forming   past   tenses   and   Participle   II   verbs   can   be
regular and irregular.
B. Lexical-morphological   classification   is   based   on   the   implicit   grammatical
meanings   of   the   verb.   According   to   the   implicit   grammatical   meaning   of
transitivity/intransitivity verbs fall into transitive and intransitive. According to the
implicit grammatical meaning of stativeness/non-stativeness verbs fall into stative
and   dynamic.   According   to   the   implicit   grammatical   meaning   of
terminativeness/non-terminativeness verbs fall into terminative and durative. This
classification is closely connected with the categories of Aspect and Phase.
C. Syntactic   classifications.   According   to   the   nature   of   predication   (primary   and
secondary)   all   verbs   fall   into   finite   and   non-finite.   According   to   syntagmatic
properties (valency) verbs can be of obligatory and optional valency, and thus they
may have some directionality or be devoid of any directionality. In this way, verbs
fall   into   the   verbs   of   directed   (to   see,   to   take,   etc.)   and   non-directed   action   (to
arrive, to drizzle, etc.)
D. Functional   classification.   According   to   their   functional   significance   verbs   can
be   notional   (with   the   full   lexical   meaning),   semi-notional   (modal   verbs,   link-
verbs), auxiliaries [2,46].
V erbs   are   the   action   words   in   a   sentence   that   describe   what   the   subject   is
doing. Along with nouns, verbs are the main part of a sentence or phrase, telling a
story   about   what   is   taking   place.   In   fact,   without   a   verb,   full   thoughts   can’t   be
properly conveyed, and even the   simplest sentences,   such as   Maria   sings,   have   one. 8Actually,   a   verb   can   be   a   sentence   by   itself,   with   the   subject,   in   most   case   you,
implied, such as, ! and ! When learning the rules of  grammar, schoolchildren are
often   taught   that   verbs   are   ‘doing’   words,   meaning   they   signify   the   part   of   the
sentence which explains the action taking place:
He   ran   away,   she   eats   chocolate   cake   on   Sundays,   the   horses   gallop   across   the
fields.   Ran,   eats   and   gallop   are the ‘action’ parts of those sentences, thus they are
the verbs.
However,   it   can   be   confusing   because   not   all   verbs   are   easily   identifiable   as
action:   I   know   your   name,   Jack   thought   about   it,   we   considered   several
applications.
These   are   non-action   verbs,   i.e.   those   that   describe   a   state   of   being,   emotion,
possession,   sense   or   opinion.   Other   non-action   verbs   include   include   love,  agree,
feel, am, and have [3,72].
How   to   Recognize   a   Verb
As you can see from the examples above, one clue to help you recognize a
verb   is   its   location   compared   to   the   subject.   Verbs   almost   always   come   after   a   noun
or   pronoun.   These   nouns   and   pronouns   are   referred   to   as   the   subject.   The
verb   thought   comes   after the noun   Jack, so   the action   Jack   (subject) was   taking
was thinking (verb).
1. Mark   eats   his   dinner   quickly.
2. We   went   to   the   market.
3. You   write   neatly   in   your   notebook.
4. They   thought   about   all   the   prizes   in   the   competition.
Here   are   some   other   ways   to   recognize   verbs   in   a   sentence: Can   I   think,   wonder,   walk,   yawn?   Yes,   so   these   are   verbs.
In   the   sentence   Mark   eats   his   dinner   quickly,   what   is   happening?   Eating   is
happening, so eating is the verb.
In   the   sentence   They   thought   about   all   the   prizes   what   is   happening?   Thought
(thinking) is happening, so thought is the verb.
Physical   Verbs   —   Definition   and   Examples
Physical   verbs   are   action   verbs.   They   describe   specific   physical   actions.   If
you  can   create   a  motion   with  your   body   or   use   a   tool   to  complete   an   action,  the
word you use to describe it is most likely a physical verb.
For    example,   Joe   sat   in	    his	    chair,   the	    dog   breathes   quickly	    after
she   chases   her ball,   and   should we   vote   in the election?   Even when the action isn’t
very active, if the action is done by the body or a tool, consider it a physical verb.
The   physical   verb   examples   in   the   following   sentences   are   in   bold   for   easy
identification.
 Let’s   run   to   the   corner   and   back.
 I   hear   the   train   coming.
 Call   me   when   you’re   finished   with   class.
Mental   Verb   —   Definition   and   Examples
Mental   verbs   have   meanings   that   are   related   to   concepts   such   as   discovering,
understanding,   thinking,   or planning.   In general,   a mental verb refers to a cognitive
state.   The   mental   verb   examples   in   the   following   sentences   are   in   bold   for   easy
identification.
 I   know   the   answer.
 She   recognized   me   from   across   the   room.
9 10 Do   you   believe   everything   people   tell   you?
States   of   Being   Verbs   —   Definition   and   Examples
Also known as linking verbs, state of being verbs describe conditions or situations
that   exist.   State   of   being   verbs   are   inactive   since   no   action   is   being   performed.
These   verbs,   forms   of   to   be,   such   as   am,   is,   are,   are   usually   complemented   by
adjectives. The state of being verbs in the following sentences are in bold for easy
identification.
Types   of   Verbs
There   are   many   types   of   verbs.   In   addition   to   the   main   categories   of   physical   verbs,
mental   verbs,   and   state   of   being   verbs,   there   are   several   other   types   of   verbs.   In
fact, there are more than ten different types of verbs that are grouped together by
function. List of all Verb Types:
Action   Verbs
Action   verbs   express   specific   actions   and   are   used   any   time   you   want   to   show
action   or   discuss   someone   doing   something.   It’s   important   to   remember   that   the
action does not have to be physical.
Action   verb   examples:
The   action   verb   examples   in   the   following   sentences   are   in   bold   for   easy  
identification.
I   run   faster   than   David. 
He does it well.
She   thinks   about   poetry   all   day   long
Transitive   Verbs 11Transitive verbs are action verbs that always express doable activities that relate or
affect someone or something else. These other things are generally direct objects,
nouns   or   pronouns   that   are   affected   by   the   verb,   though   some   verbs   can   also   take
an   indirect   object,   such   as   show,   take,   and   make.   In   a   sentence   with   a   transitive
verb,   someone   or   something   receives   the   action   of   the   verb.Transitive   verb
examples:
The   transitive   verb   examples   in   the   following   sentences   are   in   bold   for   easy
identification.
Gary   ate   the   cookies.
The transitive verb is   ate,   Gary is the subject, because it is Gary who is doing the
eating, and   the   cookies   are the direct object, because it is the cookies that are being
eaten. Other examples:
He   kicked   John.
John   punches   him.
They   sold   the   tickets.
Examples   of   verbs   used   with   both   direct   and   indirect   objects:
They   sell   him   the   tickets.
In   this   sentence,   the   tickets   are   the   direct   object   while   him   is   the   indirect   object.
Mary   baked   her   mother   a   pie. 12In   this   sentence,   a   pie   is   the   direct   object   while   her   mother   is   the   indirect   object. 13Intransitive   Verbs
Intransitive verbs are action verbs that  always express doable activities. They are
different   from   transitive   verbs   because   there   is   no   direct   object   following   an
intransitive verb.Intransitive verb examples:
The   intransitive   verb   examples   in   the   following   sentences   are   in   bold   for   easy
identification.
We   travelled   to   London.
The   intransitive   verb   is   travelled,   the   subject   is   we,   because   we   are   doing   the
travelling,   but   London   is   not   a   direct   object   because   London  is   not   receiving   the
action of the verb. Other examples:
I   sneeze   in   the   morning.
He   arrived   with   moments   to   spare.
Kathryn   sat   away   from   the   others.
John   eats   before   leaving   for   school.
The   last   example   shows   that   the   verb   can   be   both   transitive   and   intransitive
depending  on	  whether	  there	  is	  a	  direct	  object	  or	  not.	  If	  the	  sentence
read:   John   eats   the cookies   before   leaving   for   school,   would   be   transitive   as   there   is
a direct object — the cookies. 14By   the   way,   some   verbs   can   be   both   transitive   and   intransitive.   These   verbs
include: start, leave, change, live, stop.
Auxiliary Verbs
Auxiliary verbs are also known as helping verbs and are used together with a main
verb   to   show   the   verb’s tense   or   to   form   a   question   or   negative.   Common   examples
of   auxiliary   verbs   include   have,   might,   will.   These   auxiliary   verbs   give   some
context   to   the   main   verb,   for   example,   letting   the   reader   know   when   the   action   took
place.Auxiliary verb examples:
The   auxiliary   verb   examples   in   the   following   sentences   are   in   bold   for   easy
identification.
I   will   go   home   after   football   practice.
The auxiliary verb is telling us that the action of the main verb   go   is going to take
place   in  the   future  —   after   football   practice   has   ended.   If   the   auxiliary  verb  was
removed, we get the sentence:
I   go   home   after   football   practice.
In this case, there is no definite time frame for the action. The sentence suggests
that     going     home     after     football     practice     is     just     something     the
subject I generally does. Other examples:
I   may   dance   with   you   later.
We   did   consider   Bryan’s   feelings. 15Jenny   has   spoken   her   final   words.
In addition, we can sometimes use the auxiliary very before the pronoun to make a
question:
Might   you   dance   with   me   later?
Did   we   consider   Bryan’s   feelings?
Has   Jenny   spoken   her   final   words?
Also,   auxiliary   verbs   are   used   to   help   form   negative   statements,   with   the   use   of
words like not and never. These   will usually split the auxiliary and main verbs:
I   may   never   dance   with   you   again.
We   did   not   consider   Bryan’s   feelings.
Jenny   has   not   spoken   her   final   words.
Stative   Verbs
Stative verbs can be recognized because they express a state rather than an action.
They  typically  relate  to  thoughts,   emotions,  relationships,   senses,  states   of  being,
and measurements.   The best way to think about stative verbs is that they are verbs
that describe things that are not actions.   The stative verbs are all expressing a state:
A state of doubting, a state of believing, a state of wanting. These states of being
are   often   temporary.The   stative   verb   examples   in   the   following   sentences   are   in
bold for easy identification. 16The   doctor   disagrees   with   your   analysis.
Disagree   is   a   stative   verb   here,   as   it   describes   the   doctor’s   state   of   being   —
disagreement.
John   doubts   the   doctor’s   opinion.
I   believe   the   doctor   is   right.
She   wanted   another   opinion.
Modal   Verbs
Modal   verbs   are   auxiliary   verbs   that   are   used   to   express   abilities,   possibilities,
permissions, and obligations.Modal verb examples:
The   modal   verb   examples   in   the   following   sentences   are   in   bold   for   easy
identification.
He   can   shoot   a   three-point   shot   easily.
The   auxiliary   verb   can   is   expressing   an   ability,   suggesting   that   shooting   a   three-
point shot is a skill the subject possesses.
Please note that in the case of   should   and   must   in the examples below, the modal
verbs   are   expressing   obligations,   whereas   would   and   may   are   expressing
possibilities.
I   should   go   home. 17You   must   not   delay.
Sally   would   not   recommend   the   sushi.
David   may   be   late.
Phrasal   Verbs
Phrasal verbs aren’t single words; instead, they are combinations of words that are
used together to take on a different meaning to that of the original verb. There are
many examples of phrasal verbs, some of which have colloquial meanings, such as
make   up,   hand   in,   bring   up,   point   out,   look forward   to.   Each   time the   verb   takes   the
extra     word(s)     it     takes     on     a     new     meaning.     For     example,   make   without
the   up   expresses   that   something   is   being   created,   whereas   with   make   up,   the
suggestion is that there are some lies or a fantastical element to the story and   make
out   can   mean   either   to   grasp   or   see   something   difficult,   or   to   kiss
passionately.Phrasal verb examples:
The   phrasal   verb   examples   in   the   following   sentences   are   in   bold   for   easy
identification.
Mary   looked   forward   to   her   high   school   reunion.
The verb   looked   has taken on   forward to   to become a phrasal verb meaning to be
excited about or eagerly await something.
He   brought   up   the   same   points   again   and   again.
Leroy   handed   in   the   wallet   to   the   police. 18I   make   up   stories   all   the   time.
She   pointed   out   Donald’s   mistake.
Irregular   Verbs
Irregular verbs are   those that don’t take   on the regular spelling patterns of past 
simple and past participle verbs. Unfortunately, there are hundreds of irregular 
verbs in the English language. But don’t worry, while many are used often, the 
majority   are   not   in   common   usage   —   or   if   they   are,   you   will   use   them   so   often   you 
will learn them quickly. Some of the most common irregular verbs include: say, 
make, go, take, come, know and see.Irregular verb examples:
The   irregular   verb   examples   in   the   following   sentences   are   in   bold   for   easy 
identification.
I   take   my   time   when   I   go   to   the   shops   (present   tense)
I   took   my   time   when   I   went   to   the   shops   (past   tense)
Julie   makes   cake   for   the   classroom   (present   tense)
Julie   made   a   cake   for   the   classroom   (past   tense)
She   sees   a   silhouette   shaped   like   a   man   in   the   window   (present   tense)
She   saw   a   silhouette   shaped   like   a   man   in   the   window   (past   tense)
We   come   to   Aunt   Jane’s   for   Thanksgiving   each   year   (present   tense) 19We   came   to   Aunt   Jane’s   for   Thanksgiving   each   year   (past   tense).
You   should   also   remember   that   auxiliary   verbs   ‘do’   and   ‘have’   are   also   irregular 
verbs:
I   do   agree.
He   does   it   often.
We   have   done   our   homework   early.
They   do   their   homework   on   Fridays   [4,36- 39]. 201.2. Importance   of   verbs   in   sentence   construction
A verb is a part of speech used to describe motion or convey a subject  in action.
Examples of verbs include walk, shop, jump, read, etc. Verbs have specific tenses
based   on   how   you   use   them.   Present   tense   indicates   the   verb   is   occuring   in   the
moment.   Past   tense   means   the   action   has   already   taken   place.   In   addition,   future
tense  specifies  the  action  that  will   take place  at   a later   time  (future). Verbs  are a
very important part of speech because without them a sentence cannot exist. They
serve several purposes within a sentence:
Make a statement – The dog ran home. 
Help   to   ask   a   question –   Did   he   run   home? 
Give a command – Run home.
Express   action   –   The   dog   ran   after   the   ball.
Express   a   state   of   being   –   The   dog   is   tired   from   running. 
There are five types of verbs that help make a sentence:
Action verb – express an action that is either physical (talk,   run,   fall etc.)   or   mental 
(think, hope, choose, etc.)
Linking   verb   –   links   the   subject   of   a   sentence   to   another   word:   appear,   be,   feel, 
grow, look, remain, seem, smell, sound, stay, taste, etc.
Auxiliary or Helping verb – changes the tense (when something happened), voice
(relation   of   the   subject   to   the   verb   and   is   either   active   or   passive),   or   mood   (a
statement   of   fact,   what   might   or   could   be,   or   give   a   command   or   plea):   be,   do,
have, can, may, shall, was, will, etc.
Transitive verb – transfers the action from one noun to another and always has an
object   that   receives   the  action  of  the   verb  or   completes   the  meaning  of  the   verb:
Bill took the job.
Intransitive verb – doesn’t transfer action so it doesn’t have an object: The phone
broke [5,289].
Verbs are very important for language development because they allow children to
start building sentences. The choice of verb determines the grammatical form in a
sentence.   Children   who   use   more   verbs   have   more   advanced   grammatical   skills. 21Most  children use  at  least  40 verbs by the time they are 24 months old. You can
help your child learn new verbs by doing the following:
Keep a list  of the verbs your child understands and says. Keeping track will help
you know which new verbs your child is learning each month and how many.
Think about things your child likes to do. This will help you include new verbs in
their vocabulary by associating verbs with their interests.
Show your child what a verb means. Whenever possible, try to do the action while
you say the verb in a short sentence.
Repeat   the   verb   often.   Children   need  to   hear   new   words   many  times   before  they
start using the words correctly themselves.
Verbs   are   important   because   they   help   children   communicate   about   different
events   in   their   lives   by   combining   words   into   sentences.   By   understanding   what
verbs are and using some of the tips from above, you can help your child learn to
use verbs and increase their overall expressive language.
The results show that sentence onset latencies varied in relation to the presentation
of   the   verb   elicitor,   suggesting   that   sentence   processing   depends   crucially   on
having access to the information pertaining to the verb. What is more, increases in
the lexical frequency of the actual verbs used significantly reduced onset latencies
for the subject noun as expected if the verb lemmas have to be retrieved before the
sentence can be processed. Among other things, this argues against strict linearity
and   in   favor   of   hierarchical   incrementality   in   sentence   production.   Additionally,
the   results   hint   at   the   possibility   that   other   obligatory   sentence   constituents
[namely,   direct   objects (DOs)   in transitive   sentences]   may   also   have   to be   available
before the sentence can be processed [6,91].
Sentences   are   not   born fully   formed:   they   are   the   product of   a   complex   process that
requires first forming a conceptual representation that can be given linguistic form,
then   retrieving   the   right   words   related   to   that   pre-linguistic   message   and   putting
them in the right configuration, and finally converting that bundle into a series of
muscle   movements   that   will   result   in   the   outward   expression   of   the   initial
communicative   intention.   At issue is the question of whether it   is possible to start 22preparing   a   sentence   for   production   before   some   structurally   critical   sentence
constituents   are,   at   the   very   least,   conceptually   available.   That   is,   whether   a
speaker  can,   on  knowing  what   the  topic/subject   of  a  sentence  will  be  in  an  SVO
language   such   as   English,   put   the   machinery   in   motion   even   if   the   rest   of   the
utterance   is   still   un(der)determined.   It   is   not   hard   to   imagine   that,   in   certain
circumstances, the topic of the sentence may be selected before anything else is.
For example, it has been found that often more salient entities tend to be encoded
as   subjects   of   sentences,   with   flow-on   consequences   for   the   rest   of   the   sentence
(e.g., passive  vs active  voice, or  alternative perspective  descriptions). On the one
hand,   this   could   mean   that   prominence   dictates   which   entity   takes   the   topical
function of sentence subject in English, perhaps, as Bock et al. Review concludes,
because   this   prominence   correlates   with   predicability.   Such   a   scenario   is
compatible with the considerable  amount of evidence pointing to the incremental
nature of sentence production. According to this, speakers do not necessarily wait
for all the bits and pieces of a phrase or sentence to have been processed and set in
their   places   before   they   start   uttering   it.   However,   incrementality   in   sentence
production   does   not   necessarily   imply   that   processing   follows   the   sentence’s
surface form. For sentences to be more or less freely assemblable on the basis of
constituent   availability   and   in   a   strictly   linear   manner,   the   processing   of   earlier
constituents   should   not   depend   on   requirements   of   later   constituents—i.e.,   the
subject   could   constrain   verb   choices   but   it   should   not   itself   be   constrained   by   a
verb’s   specifications.   But   this   condition   does   not   always   hold:   for   example,   in
languages   with   rich   morpho-syntax   where   the   specific   relationships   between
constituents   matter   for   the   final   form   of   the   sentence   strict   linearity   would   be
unfeasible. And even in languages with more impoverished morpho-syntax such as
English, there are dependencies between different sentence constituents that would
be expected to limit the extent to which some parts of a sentence can be processed
separately from other later coming parts [7,31-33].
That is, there are underlying structural relationship between different constituents
in   a   sentence   that   can   only   be   properly   established   once   all   the   constituents   in 23question are available to the speaker. This may explain why much of the evidence
for   linear   incrementality   concerns   optional   constituents   such   as   adjectives   and
prepositional modifiers of a noun , while research on full sentences has tended to
support   a   more   hierarchical   incrementality   where   the   processing   of   certain
constituents does seem to depend on the processing of later ones with which they
are   structurally   related.   Hierarchical   incrementality   presupposes   that   speakers
cannot start processing a sentence on the basis of a single constituent, but instead
need   to   have   at   least   a   rough   idea   of   the   overall   relationships   between   the
constituents   within   that   sentence.   A   strong   version   of   the   hierarchical
incrementality   account   would   require   access   to   specific   lexical   items   such   as   the
verb   to   enable   building   the   initial   sentence   outline   since   some   structural
relationships   are   based   on   individual   word   specifications   (e.g.,   nothing   in   the
semantics  of   “donate”  and  “give”   explains  why   the   receiver  can   be  an  object  for
the latter but not the former: “the gentleman gave/ ∗ donated the library his books”)
[8,12].
That   would   be   lemma-driven   scaffolding.   A   weaker   version,   structure-driven
scaffolding,   proposes   that   the   basic   hierarchical   structure   could   be   derived   from   the
more   abstract   conceptual   representation   of   the   communicative   intention,   before
specific   lexical   items   are   retrieved.   Both   possibilities   are   compatible   with   the
finding   that,   when   describing   a   picturable   event,   speakers   appear   to   use   the   first
few   hundred   milliseconds,   around   300–400,   scanning   the   whole   scene   before
settling   on   the   component   of   the   scene   that   would   be   produced   first.   It   is   also
possible   that   both   lemma-driven   and   structure-driven   scaffolding   operate   during
sentence production—syntactic processing is not monolithic, and it may allow for
different   circumstances   favoring   the   engagement   of   different   mechanisms.   Given
the central role verbs play in sentences, one straightforward prediction that can be
derived   from   hierarchical   incrementality   is   that   either   the   verb   or   the   conceptual
representation underlying it (which will be referred to as the “action” henceforth1)
should be essential for initiating sentence production. 24That is, it should not be possible to start preparing a sentence before the action is
known since it is what ties the different participants in an event together and what
will   ultimately,   in   the   form   of   a   verb,   underpin   the   relationships   between   the
different sentence constituents. In fact, early psycholinguistic accounts of sentence
production assumed verbs played a critical role in the generation of a sentence by
being   responsible   for   its   basic   structural   shape—an   assumption   that   has   found
some empirical support from, among others, eye tracking and priming experiments
found facilitation during picture description when the action in the sentence to be
produced   had   been   linguistically   primed,   but   not   when   the   subject   had   been
visually   primed,   suggesting   the   verb   plays   a   major   role   in   sentence   planning [9,58].
And Hwang and Kaiser found that the action region was fixated before the subject
region during sentence production in English. In sum, there are reasons to believe
that   the   action/verb   is   processed   early   during   sentence   production.   Densities   and
quartiles of subject noun latencies in ms relative to onset of the Person image for
the three Verb Types and the four Presentation conditions of interest: two based on
presentation   order   of   the   Person   and   the   Action,   and   two   based   on   presentation
order of the Person and the Thing. The length of the shapes reflects the spread of
latencies   and   the   width   is   an   indication   of   the   number   of   observations   at   any
particular   latency   point.   The   lines   divide   the   data   into   the   longest   25%   onset
latencies, the second longest 25% onset latencies, etc. The diamond shapes denote
the mean onset latencies of each group. not all published evidence points to verbs
being   necessary   to   start   uttering   a   sentence.   Schriefers   et   al.  Found   a   semantic
interference effect on the verb when the sentence  was Verb-Subject  (in German),
but   not   when  it   was  Subject-Verb,  which  implies  processing  of   the  verb  was  not
interfering with processing of the subject when the latter was uttered first.
Additionally,   while   some   authors   have   found   that   the   verb’s   internal   arguments
[i.e., direct objects and subjects of   unaccusative   verbs]   depend on verb retrieval for
their processing, some of this research also finds that typical subjects (i.e., those in
transitive   and   unergative   sentences)   do   not.   In   particular,   Momma   et   al.   Have
found   that   the   time   it   takes   to   start   uttering   a   sentence   increases   when   there   is 25semantic interference affecting the verb, but this only happens for sentences where
the   DO   precedes   the   verb,   or   when   the   subject   is   semantically   more   related   to   a   DO
than   to   a   typical   subject,   but   not   when   the   first   constituent   in   the   sentence   is   a
typical sentence subject. This difference between the reliance on verb access of the
two   types   of   verbal   arguments   could   be   explained   by   the   closer   relationship
between   verbs   and   their   internal   arguments   than   between   verbs   an   their   external
arguments, but it puts into question the notion that verbs are always necessary for
overall sentence processin [10,117]. 26CONCLUSION   ON   CHAPTER   I
In   conclusion,   verbs   are   essential   components   of   language,   serving   as   the
action   words   that   convey   meaning   and   drive   communication.   They   encompass   a
wide range of functions, from expressing actions and states to indicating time and
mood.   Understanding   the   role   and   usage   of   verbs   is   fundamental   for   effective
communication   and   language   proficiency.   By   mastering   verbs,   individuals   can
enhance their ability to express themselves clearly and accurately in any linguistic
context.   Furthermore,   verbs   play   a   crucial   role   in   constructing   sentences   and
conveying complex ideas. They provide the foundation upon which sentences  are
built,   allowing   speakers   and   writers   to   express   relationships   between   subjects,
objects, and various elements of discourse. Verbs also contribute to the dynamics
of   language,   enabling   speakers   to   convey   nuances   of   meaning,   such   as   tense,
aspect, and mood. Moreover, through the use of verbs, individuals can evoke vivid
imagery,   evoke   emotions,   and   engage   readers   or   listeners   on   a   deeper   level.   In
essence, verbs are the   dynamic engines that propel language   forward, making them
indispensable tools for effective communication across cultures and contexts. 27CHAPTER   II.   THE   SYSTEM   OF   THE   VERB   IS   REPRESENTED   BY   TWO 
SETS OF FORMS FINITE AND NON-FINITE FORMS.
2.1. The   non-finite   forms:   infinitive,   gerund,   present   participle
The   non-finite   forms   (the   verbals)   are   the   Infinitive,   the   Gerund   and   the
Participle. They are not formally related to the doer of the action: the relationship
is   semantic,   that’s   why   they   have   no   categories   which   formally   mark   the
relationship   between   the   action   and   the   doer   of   the   action   (no   person   and   no
number), no categories of predication (the tense, the mood) – they can’t be used as
the predicate of the sentence, however the Infinitive and the Gerund can generate
one-member sentences:   No smoking! Why go there? To think of it! The Infinitive
has three grammatical categories: voice, time correlation, aspect. The Gerund and
Participle   I   have   two   grammatical   categories:   voice   and   time   correlation.   Non-
finite forms combine   nominal and verbal features – they are   partly verbs and partly
names   (adjectives).   This   contiguity   explains   the   attempts   to   refer   the   non-finite
forms to certain classes according to their functioning. Thus in some grammars the
Infinitive   was   referred   to   the   noun,   the   Participle   –   to   the   adjective.   As   to   the
Gerund   it   was   a   controversial   point   as   it   exists   only   in   English   and   in   Spanish.
Verbids are intermediary in many of the lexical grammatical features between the
verb and non-processual  parts of speech. They render processes  as peculiar kinds
of   substances   and   properties   while   the   finite   forms   serve   only   one   syntactic
function (finite predicate). The non-finite serves  various  syntactic  functions  other
than  that   of  the   finite  predicate.  The  opposition  between   the   finite  and  non-finite
forms   of   the   verb   creates   special   grammatical   categories.   The   differential   feature   of
the   opposition   is   constituted   by   the   expression   of   verbal   time   and   mood.   The
verbid has no immediate means of expressing time, mood and categorial semantics
and is therefore, the weak member of the opposition. This category can be called
the   category of finitude:   “Have you ever had anything caught in your head – Have
you ever had anything that was caught in your head” [11,264]. 28The verbids, unable to express the predicative meanings of time and mood,
still express secondary or potential predication. The opposition of the finite verbs
and the verbids is based on the expression of the functions of full predication and
semi-predication:  while the finite verbs express predication in complete form, the
function of the verbids is to express semi-predication building up semi-predicative
complexes.   The   English   verbids   include   4   forms:   the   infinitive,   the   gerund,   the
present and past participle. The infinitive is the non-finite form of the verb which
combines  the  properties  of   the  verb  and noun.  Serving  as  the  verbal  name  of   the
process,   the   English   infinitive   exists   in   two   presentation   forms:   with   and
without   to: to-infinitive (or marked) – bare infinitive (or unmarked). The gerund   is
the non-finite form of the verb which like the infinitive combines the properties of
a   verb   and   those   of   the   noun.   The   gerund   also   serves   as   the   verbal   name   of   the
process   but   its   substantive   quality   is   more   stronger   pronounced   than   that   of   the
infinitive. The gerund performs the functions of all the types of notional sentence
parts.   The   present   participle   is   the   non-finite   form   of   the   verb   that   combines   the
properties   of   the   verb   with   those   of   the   adjective   and   adverb.   It   is   homonymous
with gerund and is distinguishing the same grammatical categories of retrospective
coordinations   and   voice.   The   self-positional   present   participle   performs   the
functions  of  the  predicative  the  attribute, the adverbial  modifier  of  various types.
The past participle is the   non-finite form of   the   verb which combines the   properties
of the verb with those of the adjective, serving as the qualifying processual name.
This form has no paradigm of its own. The past unlike the present participle has no
distinct   features,   characteristic   of   the   adverb.   The   main   functions   of   the   past
participle   in   the   sentence   are   those   of   the   attribute   and   the   predicative.   The
infinitive – gerund correlation.   Both forms are substance-processual but there is a
clear-out   difference   between   these   forms.   Gerund   has   a   more   substantive   nature
than the infinitive [12,163].
It   is   also   more   abstract.   Hence,   these   forms   don’t   repeat   but   complement   each 
other:   “Seeing   and   talking   to   people   made   him   tired.”   –   “It   made   him   tired   to   see 29and  talk   to  so   many   people”.   In   the   first   example  the   person   is   talking  about   are
general features of   someone’s behavior.   The   second is one   particular   occasion. The
infinitive has more dynamic, more actional  character, compared to corresponding
gerundial   expressions:   “For   them   to   have   arrived   so   early   such   a   surprise!”   –
“Their   early arrival   was so surprising.” This triple   correlation   being of   a   systematic
nature enables us to interpret it in terms of a special  lexico-grammatical category
of  processual  representation. The infinitive and  its  phrase is  dynamic, the gerund
and its   phrase   is semi-dynamic   and the   verbal   noun and   its   phrase   is static. Another
category within substantival  verbids is the category of modal representation. This
category   marks   the   infinitive   in   contrasts   to   the   gerund   and   is   revealed   in   the
infinitive having a modal force: “This is a kind of piece desired by all.” – “A kind
of   piece   that   should   be   desired.”   “Is   there   any   hope   for   us   to   meet   her   in   the
university?”   –   “A   hope   that   we   may   meet   this   person   in   the   university.”   The
gerund – participle correlation.   At present time there is a discussion if the present
participle and the gerund constitute different verbids or if they present one and the
same form. These forms are outwardly the same when viewed in isolation. And it
is   not   by   chance   that   in   the   American   linguistic   tradition   they   are   recognized   as   one
integral   verb   –   ing.   In   treating   the   –ing   forms   as   constituting   one   integral   verbal
entity opposed on the one hand to the infinitive and on the other hand to the past
participle, appeal is naturally made to the alternating views of the possessive and
the common-objective nounal element in the role of the subject of the –ing form.
They are mostly observed in various positions of the sentence:
I   felt   annoyed   at   his   (gerund)   him   (participle)   failing   to   see   my   point   at   once.
I   saw   him   crossing   the   street   /   his   coming.   I   insist   on   him   being   invited   [13,159]. 302.2. The   verb   in   its   finite   form   possesses   the   number,   tense,   aspect,   voice   and
mood
The form of the verb may show whether the agent expressed by the subject
is the doer of the action or the recipient of the action (John broke the vase-the vase
was broken). The objective relations between the action and the subject  or object
of   the   action   find   their   expression   in   language   as   the   grammatical   category   of
voice. Therefore, the category of voice reflects the objective relations between the
action   itself   and   the   subject   or   object   of   the   action.   The   category   of   voice   is
realized through the opposition Active voice: Passive voice. The realization of the
voice   category   is   restricted   because   of   the   implicit   grammatical   meaning   of
transitivity/intransitivity.   In   accordance   with   this   meaning,   all   English   verbs   should
fall into   transitive   and intransitive.   However,   the   classification turns out to be   more
complex and comprises 6 groups:
1. Verbs   used   only   transitively:   to   mark,   to   raise;
2. Verbs   with   the   main   transitive   meaning:   to   see,   to   make,   to   build;
3. Verbs   of   intransitive   meaning   and   secondary   transitive   meaning.   A   lot   of
intransitive verbs may develop a secondary transitive meaning: They laughed me
into agreement; He danced the girl out of the room;
4. Verbs of a double nature, neither of the meanings are the leading one, the verbs
can be used both transitively and intransitively: to drive home-to drive a car;
5. Verbs   that   are   never   used   in   the   Passive   Voice:   to   seem,   to   become;
6. Verbs   that   realize   their   passive   meaning   only   in   special   contexts:   to   live,   to
sleep, to sit, to walk, to jump.
Some   scholars   admit   the   existence   of   Middle,   Reflexive   and   Reciprocal   voices.
Middle Voice" - the verbs primarily transitive may develop an intransitive middle
meaning:   That   adds   a   lot;   The   door   opened;   The   book   sells   easily;   The   dress
washes well. 31"Reflexive Voice":  He dressed;  He washed-  the  subject  is  both the agent  and the
recipient   of   the   action   at   the   same   time.   It   is   always   possible   to   use   a   reflexive
pronoun in this case: He washed himself.
"Reciprocal   voice”:   They   met;   They   kissed-   it   is   always   possible   to   use   a
reciprocal pronoun here: They kissed each other.
We cannot, however, speak of different voices, because all these meanings are not
expressed morphologically.
The   category   of   tense
The category of tense is a verbal category that reflects the objective category
of   time.   The   essential   characteristic   of   the   category   of   tense   is   that   it   relates   the
time of the action, event or state of affairs referred to in the sentence to the time of
the utterance (the time of the utterance being "now" or the present  moment). The
tense   category   is   realized   through   the   oppositions.   The   binary   principle   of
oppositions remains the basic one in the correlation of the forms that represent the
grammatical category of tense. The present moment is the main temporal plane of
verbal   actions.   Therefore,   the   temporal   dichotomy   may   be   illustrated   by   the
following   graphic   representation   (the   arrows   show   the   binary   opposition)
Generally   speaking,   the   major   tense-distinction   in   English   is   undoubtedly   that
which  is  traditionally  described  as   an  opposition  of   past::present.   But  this  is   best
regarded as  a contrast  of  past::  non-past. Quite a lot of  scholars  do not  recognize
the   existence   of   future   tenses,   because   what   is   described   as   the   'future'   tense   in
English is realized by means of auxiliary verbs will and shall.
Although   it   is   undeniable   (неоспоримый)   that   will   and   shall   occur   in   many
sentences that refer to the future, they also   occur in sentences that do not. And they
do   not   necessarily   occur   in   sentences   with   a   future   time   reference.   That   is   why
future tenses are often treated as partly modal.
The   Category   of   Aspect 32The category of aspect is a linguistic representation of the objective category
of   Manner   of   Action.   It   is   realized   through   the   opposition   Continuous::Non-
Continuous   (Progressive::Non-Progressive).   The   realization   of   the   category   of
aspect   is   closely   connected   with   the   lexical   meaning   of   verbs.   There   are   some
verbs in English that do not normally occur with progressive aspect, even in those
contexts   in   which   the   majority   of   verbs   necessarily   take   the   progressive   form.
Among   the   so-called   ‘non-progressive’   verbs   are   think,   understand,   know,   hate,
love,  see,   taste,   feel,  possess,   own,  etc.   The  most   striking   characteristic   that   they
have in common is the fact that they are ‘stative’ - they refer to a state of affairs,
rather than to an action, event or process. It should be observed, however, that all
the   ‘non-progressive'   verbs   take   the   progressive   aspect   under   particular
circumstances.   As   the   result   of   internal   transposition   verbs   of   non-progressive
nature   can   be   found   in   the   Continuous   form:   Now   I'm   knowing   you.   Generally
speaking   the   Continuous   form   has   at   least   two   semantic   features   -   duration   (the
action   is   always   in   progress)   and   definiteness   (the   action   is   always   lim   ited   to   a
definite   point   or   period   of   time).   In   other   words,   the   purpose   of   the   Continuous
form is to serve as a frame which makes the process  of  the action more concrete
and isolated.
Non-finite verbs are different from finite verbs because you cannot use them as the
main   verbs   in   clauses   all   the   time.   These   verbs   do   not   always   agree   with   the
subject or first argument in terms of number, gender, and person. Non-finite verb
forms  are  also   known  for  lacking  mood  and  voice.   They  only  function  as  nouns,
adverbs, and adjectives. Sometimes, we combine them with a finite verb for verb
tense.   All   verbs   except   for   modal   auxiliaries   have   non-finite   forms.   A   non-finite
phrase or clause is a group of words with non-finite verbs. There are three types of
non-finite  verbs  in  the  English   language.  I’ll  break  each   down  separately  [14,15-
19].
Gerunds 32A   gerund   is   a   verb   in   its   -ing   form   that   acts   as   a   noun .   Some   examples
include   reading,   buying,   shopping, and   singing. Note   that   this   non-finite   verb is not
used   in   the   continuous   tense   despite   its   identical   appearance.   That’s   because   it
doesn’t   have   auxiliary   verbs   before   it.   Here   are   some   non-finite   verb   examples   I
whipped up to show in sentences. These statements contain longer gerund phrases
instead of gerunds standing on their own.
 I   enjoy   singing   at   the   top   of   my   lungs   with   my   friends.
 Swimming in the pool during summer is my favorite. 
We   can   also   create   gerunds   using   helping   verbs.   For   example:
 Having   written   a   letter   of   intent   makes   you   more   qualified.
 Being   wounded   is   a   fact   of   life.
The   doer   of   the   gerund   can   be   clear   or   unclear   in   the   sentence.   Here   are   two 
sentence examples.
 Denise   likes   drinking   Shiraz.   (Denise   themselves   drinks   Shiraz)
 I   was   amazed   by   their   singing.
Gerunds are   excellent   if   you   want   to   keep   your   sentences   more   concise.   They   serve
as   alternatives   to   nouns,   prepositions,   and   articles   that   make   your   sentence   jolty.
For example:
 The   use   of   papyrus,   wood,   and   animal   hides   for   writing   was   an   ancient 
practice.
 Using   papyrus,   wood,   and   animal   hides   for   writing   was   an   ancient   practice.
Infinitives
The  non-finite   infinitive  verb  is  the  basic   dictionary  form   of  a  verb.  These  verbs
usually   have   the   word   to   before   them.   One   example   of   an   infinitive   is   to   come.
Infinitives have several purposes. You can write them beside verbs, as a modifier,
as noun phrases, or when asking questions. 33Noun   Phrase
As   noun   phrases,   infinitive   phrases   can   be   subjects   or   objects   of   sentences.   For
example:
 To   die   by   your   side   is   such   a   heavenly   way   to   die   [The  Smiths]
In   this   sentence,   the   infinitive   phrase   to die   by   your   side   functions   as   the   sentence’s
subject. Notice how there’s no specified doer when we use infinitives as subjects
of sentences. Here’s another example.
 To   err   is   human.
Infinitives   can   also   function   as   objects   of   sentences.   For   example:
 It   was   lovely   to   see   you.
Infinitives   With   Other   Verbs-Infinitives   are   often   used   with   other   verbs.   For
example:
 I   want   to   read   classic   books.
 She   never   fails   to   make   me   laugh.
You   can   also   use   many   infinitives   with   one   main   verb   in   a   sentence.   For   example:
 I   plan   to   buy   a   dress   for   an   event,   to   cook dinner   for   my   daughter,   and to
spend time with my partner.
The   infinitives   to   buy,   to   cook,   and   to   spend   all   follow   the   verb   plan.   The   most
common verbs that come before infinitives are convince, try, able, want, and like.
Infinitives   as   Modifiers-Infinitives   can   also   function   as   adjectives   and 
adverbs. For example:
 A  recommendation to	  improve   the   public	  railway	  system	  in	  the   city. 
(adjective)
 Lovely   to   look   at,   nice   to   hold.   But   if   you   break   it,   consider   it   sold.   (adverb) 34Bare Infinitive-Sometimes, infinitives occur without the word to.   This is called the
bare infinitive. Consider this example.
 We   play   hide   and   seek   all   the   time.
 I   can   play  chess.
Both sentences have the word play.   But in the first sentence, I show it as a present
tense verb. However, the second sentence I have it as a bare infinitive. These non-
finite   verbs   can   be   in   verb   chains   like   can   and   should.   Other   auxiliary   verbs
include   may, might, would, etc. To know if it’s a bare infinitive or a present tense
verb,  change   the   subject   of   the  sentence   and   see   how   it   affects   the   sentence.   For
example:
 She   plays   hide   and   seek   all   the   time.
 She   can   play   chess.
You can also use infinitives for elliptical questions. Use the question words how,  
when, or what plus the infinitive. For example:
 I   don’t   know   how   to   cook.
Participles-A   participle   is   a   type   of   non-finite   verb   that   modifies   a   noun,   verb, noun
phrase, or verb phrase. In other words, it functions like an adjective or adverb. A
participle   is   divided   into   two:   present   participles   and   past   participles.   Present
Participle-The present participle is the -ing   form of the verb. Do not confuse these
non-finite   verbs   with   gerunds   since   gerunds   act   as   nouns   and   present   participles
don’t. For example:
 Listening   to   music   is   my   hobby.
 The   listening   device   is   in   the   multimedia   room.
In the first sentence,   sleeping acts as a noun. Therefore, it is a gerund. The second
sentence  shows   listening   as	  a	  present	  participle	  because	  it	  modifies	  the
word   device. Present participles in participial phrases have three major functions.
First, it can be an adjective phrase describing a noun phrase. For example: 35 The   woman   wearing   eyeglasses   is   my   mother.
It can also be an adverb. In the sentence below, the subject is the same as the main
clause.
 Wearing   my   pajamas,   I   opened   my   bedroom   door   and   found   my   sister
crying.
Lastly,   present   participles   function   as   general   modifiers   in   sentences.   For   example:
 Broadly   speaking,   the   event   was   enjoyable   and   successful.
Putting a participle phrase upfront helps you create sentence structures that engage
readers.   You   might   also   see   present   participles   with   auxiliary   verbs   before   them.
This verb form helps complete continuous tenses. For example:
 I   was   sleeping   when   Joey   knocked.   (past   continuous)
Past Participle-The past participle   looks like   the simple past tense   of   the   verb if it’s
regular. Some examples include   lived, danced,   and   cooked. But   some   verbs change
their  spelling	  or	  stay	  the	  same	  for	  their	  past	  participle	  forms.	  For
example,   write   becomes   written,   and   become   stays   the   same.   Often,   a   past
participle   initiates   a   participle   phrase   and   functions   as   an   adjective.   Below   is   an
example
 The   sunken   ship   was   discovered   last   year.
Past   participles   can   also   act   as   adverbs.   Below   is   an  example.
 Considered   the best restaurant in the world, Geranium is a meat-free zone in 
Copenhagen.
Non-finite clauses  are  clauses   that  have  non-finite  verbs  or  verbs  with  no tenses.
We usually find them in subordinate or dependent clauses. For example:
 I   ate   bacon   and   eggs   before  leaving. 36 After spending all his money on cars and trips, Joel finally decided to
invest.
 In   order   to   travel   to   another   country,   you   must   have   a   passport.
Finite and Non-finite Verbs Can Look Identical-It’s okay if you can’t differentiate
between   finite   and   non-finite   verbs   the   first   time   as   this   skill   requires   thorough
analysis. Here’s an example of how these two types of verbs look the same.
 I   fried   chicken   because   fried   chicken   is   my  favorite.
In this sentence, the first fried   is finite. It tells us that the statement is in the simple
past   tense.   But   the   second   fried   is   a   past   participle   functioning   as   an
adjective.   Fried   modifies the word chicken. Learn Non-Finite Verbs-My guide has
shown you the definition and types of non-finite verbs. A non-finite verb is not the
main   verb   in   an   independent   clause   because   it   is   not   the   action   in   the   sentence.
Learning the different types of non-finite verbs and how to use them will help you
create shorter, better-flowing sentences [15,83-85]. 37CONCLUSION   ON   CHAPTER  II
In conclusion, the system of verbs is represented by two sets of forms: finite
and non-finite forms.   Finite verbs function as the main verbs in sentences, carrying
tense, mood, and agreement markers to indicate grammatical relationships within a
sentence.   On   the   other   hand,   non-finite   verbs   lack   tense,   person,   and   number
markers, and they serve various functions such as expressing actions as infinitives,
participles,   and   gerunds.   Understanding   the   distinction   between   finite   and   non-
finite   forms   is   crucial   for   mastering   the   nuances   of   sentence   structure   and
expressing complex ideas effectively. By grasping the interplay between these two
sets   of   forms,   language   users   can   navigate   the   intricacies   of   verb   usage   with
confidence and precision. This duality in the system of verbs offers flexibility and
richness   to   language,   allowing   speakers   and   writers   to   convey   a   wide   range   of
meanings   and   nuances.   Finite   forms   provide   clarity   and   specificity,   indicating
when   an   action   occurs   and   who   is   performing   it,   while   non-finite   forms   offer
versatility, enabling expressions of purpose, possibility, and abstraction. Together,
they form the backbone of sentence structure, facilitating clear communication and
conveying   subtleties   of   meaning.   Mastery   of   both   finite   and   non-finite   forms
empowers language users to craft compelling narratives, articulate complex ideas,
and   engage   with   diverse   audiences   effectively.   In   essence,   the   dynamic   interplay
between   finite   and   non-finite   forms   underscores   the   richness   and   depth   of
language, highlighting its capacity for expression and communication. 38CONCLUSION
In  conclusion,  the  grammatical   categories  of  the  verb  are vital   components
of   language   structure,   allowing   for   precise   communication   and   nuanced
expression.   These   categories   include   tense,   aspect,   mood,   voice,   and   agreement,
each   serving   a   distinct   function   in   conveying   meaning   within   a   sentence.   By
understanding   and   manipulating   these   categories,   language   users   can   convey   not
only   the   timing   and   nature   of   actions   but   also   the   speaker's attitude,   the   relationship
between   participants,   and   the   focus   of   the   discourse.   Mastery   of   verb   categories
enables speakers and writers to navigate the complexities of language with finesse,
crafting   coherent   and   impactful   messages   across   various   contexts.   Thus,   a
comprehensive   grasp   of   the   grammatical   categories   of   the   verb   is   essential   for
effective communication and language proficiency. Furthermore, the grammatical
categories   of   the   verb   provide   a   framework   for   organizing   and   structuring
language,   facilitating   comprehension   and   interpretation   for   both   speakers   and
listeners.   Tense   allows for the expression of time relationships, aspect indicates the
manner in which actions unfold over time, mood conveys the speaker's attitude or
certainty regarding an action, voice specifies  the relationship between the subject
and   the   action,   and   agreement   ensures   grammatical   coherence   within   a   sentence.
Together,   these   categories   contribute   to   the   richness   and   precision   of   language,
enabling the   expression   of a   wide   range   of   ideas and experiences.   By mastering the
grammatical   categories   of   the   verb,   individuals   can   wield   language   with   greater
fluency,   clarity,   and   effectiveness,   enhancing   their   ability   to   communicate
effectively in diverse linguistic contexts. In essence, the grammatical categories of
the   verb   serve   as   pillars   of   language   structure,   facilitating   the   conveyance   of
meaning and fostering meaningful communication among speakers and listeners. 39THE   LIST   OF   USED  LITERATURE
1. Mirziyoyev,   SH.   M.   Yangi   Ozbekistonda   Erkin va   Farovon   Yashaylik.   Toshkent
2021, 91-p.
2. Allum   P.   H.,   Wheeldon   L.   Scope   of   Lexical   access   in   spoken   sentence
production:   implications   for   the   conceptual-syntactic   interface.   J.   Exp   (2009),   46-p.
3. Antón-Méndez  I.. Visual  salience effects on speaker  choices:  direct or  indirect
influences on linguistic processing? (2017), 72-p.
4. Barr   D.   J.,   Levy   R.,   Scheepers   C.,   Tily   H.   J..   Random   effects   structure   for
confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. J. Mem. Lang (2013), 36-39p.
5. Bates   D.,   Maechler   M.,   Bolker   B.,   Walker   S..   Fitting   linear   mixed-effects
models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw (2015), 289-p.
6. Bock  K., Ferreira  V.  S.  “Syntactically  speaking,”  in The  Oxford Handbook  of
Language   Production,   eds   Goldrick   M.   A.,   Ferreira   V.   S.   (Oxford:   Oxford
University Press; ), (2014), 91-p.
7. Bock K., Irwin D. E., Davidson D. J. “Putting first things first,” in The Interface
of   Language,   Vision   and   Action.   Eye   Movements   and   the   Visual   World,   eds
Henderson J. M., Ferreira F. (New York, NY: Psychology Press; ),(2004), 31-33p.
8. Bock   K.,   Levelt   W.   J.   M.   “Language   production.   Grammatical   encoding,”   in
Handbook   of   Psycholinguistics,   ed.   Gernsbacher   M.   (New   York,   NY:   Academic
Press; ), (1994), 12-p.
9. Bornkessel   I.,   Schlesewsky   M.   The   extended   argument   dependency   model:   a
neurocognitive   approach   to   sentence   comprehension   across   languages.   Psychol
(2006), 58-p.
10. Brown-Schmidt S., Konopka A. E. Little houses and casas pequeñas: message
formulation and syntactic form in unscripted speech with speakers of English and
Spanish (2008), 117-p. 4011. Brown-Schmidt S., Konopka A. E. Processes of incremental message planning
during conversation. Psychonom. Bull (2015), 264-p.
12. Burzio   L.   Italian   Syntax:   a   Government-Binding   Approach.   Dordrecht:   D.
Reidel Publishing Company (1986), 163-p.
13. Costa A., Strijkers K., Martin C., Thierry G. The time course of word retrieval
revealed by event-related brain potentials during overt speech (2009), 159-p.
14. Fleischman,   Suzanne,   The   Future   in   Thought   and   Action,   Cambridge   Univ.
Press (1982), 15-19p.
15. Gideon   Goldenberg,   "On   Verbal   Structure   and   the   Hebrew   Verb",   in:   idem,
Studies in Semitic Linguistics, Jerusalem: Magnes Press (1998), 83-85p.

Grammatical categories of the verb

Sotib olish
  • O'xshash dokumentlar

  • Analysis of English and Uzbek poetry
  • Sohaga oid matnlar tarjimasidagi muammolar
  • Tarjimaning lingvistik va nolingvistik aspektlari
  • Ilmiy-texnikaviy tarjima
  • Tarjima nazaryasi va amaliyoti tarixi

Xaridni tasdiqlang

Ha Yo'q

© Copyright 2019-2025. Created by Foreach.Soft

  • Balansdan chiqarish bo'yicha ko'rsatmalar
  • Biz bilan aloqa
  • Saytdan foydalanish yuriqnomasi
  • Fayl yuklash yuriqnomasi
  • Русский